Running head: MEMORY SKILLS 1
Hamilton, H. (2011). Memory skills of deaf learners: Implications and applications. American Annals of the Deaf
156 (4), xxxxxx.
Memory skills of deaf learners: Implications and applications
Harley Hamilton
Georgia Institute of Technology
MEMORY SKILLS 2
Abstract
This paper will review research on working memory and short-term memory abilities of
deaf individuals delineating strengths and weaknesses. The areas of memory reviewed include
weaknesses such as sequential recall, processing speed, attention, and memory load. Strengths
include free recall, visuospatial recall, imagery and dual encoding. Phonological encoding, and
rehearsal appear to be strengths when these strategies are employed. The implications of the
strengths and weaknesses for language learning and educational achievement are discussed.
Research questions are posed and remedial and compensatory classroom applications are
suggested.
MEMORY SKILLS 3
Introduction
Some psychologists consider working memory (WM) and short-term memory (STM) to
be synonymous and representing a memory store that is constrained both by the number of items
that can be maintained and the length of time these items can be active. Denh (2008)
distinguishes WM and STM as follows. STM passively holds information. WM actively
processes it.
Research over the past two decades has demonstrated that performance on WM and STM
tasks is highly predictive of academic achievement in areas such as:
reading (Cain, 2006; Cain & Oakhill, 2006)
language comprehension (Engle, Carullo, & Collins, 1991)
mathematics (Geary, Hoard, Nugent, & Byrd-Craven, 2007; Jarvis & Gathercole,
2003).
science (Gathercole & Alloway, 2008; Gathercole & Pickering, 2000;
Gathercole, Pickering, S.J., Knight, C. & Stegmann, 2004, Jarvis & Gathercole,
2003)
Long-term memory, the repository of knowledge, is able to acquire very little information
without these gateways functioning properly (Denh, 2008). Deficits in WM and STM may
potentially limit students’ ability to learn and function in school (Alloway, Gathercole,
Kirkwood, & Elliott, 2009). Although it is not yet well understood how these contribute to
academic skills, it has been suggested that learning is hampered or fails when task demands
exceed memory capacity (Ayres, 2009; Gathercole, Lamont & Alloway, 2006, Reber &
Kotovsky, 1997).
MEMORY SKILLS 4
For children with disabilities, deficits in memory processes have been found (Alloway &
Gathercole, 2006; Pickering, 2006). For example, children with reading disabilities have specific
difficulties in retrieving speech-based codes and monitoring attentional processes (Swanson,
Zheng, & Jerman, 2009). WM deficits have also been described for groups of children who
exhibit :
mathematical learning disabilities (Geary, Hoard, Nugent, & Byrd-Craven, 2007)
intellectual disabilities (Henry & Winfield, 2010)
speech and language impairments (Archibald & Gathercole, 2006)
autism (Bennetto, Pennington, & Rogers, 1996)
attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Rapport, et al,, 2008).
A greater understanding of the memory limitations in children can ultimately inform the
development of communication and classroom practices and may result in improved language
learning and school outcomes. To date, however, relatively little research has been conducted to
develop and evaluate innovative approaches to minimize memory demands in the classroom
(Gathercole & Alloway, 2008) or directly improve memory for students who are most at risk for
communicative or academic failure (Holmes, Gathercole, & Dunning, 2009; Klingberg et. al.,
2005; Swanson, Kehler, & Jerman, 2010). The low levels of academic achievement common for
deaf individuals (Gallaudet Research Institute, 1996; Marschark, 2006; Meadow-Orleans, 2001;
Moores, 2001, 2003; Traxler, 2000) may also have a basis in memory processes (Blair, 1957;
Marschark, et al., 2009).
This paper will review research on working memory and short-term memory abilities of
deaf individuals. In this paper, “deaf” refers to those individuals with a hearing loss of 70
decibels or higher. This review focuses on deaf signing individuals. The areas of memory
MEMORY SKILLS 5
reviewed include those in which deaf individuals exhibit deficiencies and strengths. Deficiencies
refer to areas in which deaf individuals perform less well than hearing individuals and strengths
refer to areas in which deaf individuals perform equal to or better than hearing individuals.
Deficiencies include sequential recall, processing speed, attention, and memory load. Areas of
strength include free recall, visuospatial recall, imagery, and dual encoding. Areas which
emerge as strengths when the particular strategy is employed include phonological encoding and
rehearsal. The implications of the deficiencies and strengths for language learning and
educational achievement are discussed. The results of the literature review then form the basis
for suggested remedial and compensatory activities to enhance learning. Research questions are
also delineated regarding the proposed educational applications.
Memory Skills of Deaf Learners
Memory Deficits and their Effects on Language Learning and Academic Achievement
Sequential memory.
Sequential memory is recall or processing of a list or other stimulus such as a sentence in
the same order as it was presented. Bebko (1984) has noted that deaf individuals have greater
difficulty with sequential memory processing tasks than hearing individuals. For deaf
individuals, deficits have been found in comparison to hearing chronologically age-matched
peers for immediate sequential recall of lists of:
digits (Blair, 1957; Flaherty & Moran, 2004; Koo, Crain , LaSasso, Eden,
2008; Olsson & Furth, 1966; Parasnis, Samar, Betger, & Sathe, 1996;
Pintner & Patterson, 1917; Tomlinson-Keasey & Smith-Winberry, 1990)
printed words (Flaherty & Moran, 2004; Hanson, 1982; Krakow & Hanson,
1985)
MEMORY SKILLS 6
pictures (Blair, 1957; Bebko, 1984; Bebko & McKinnon, 1990; Campbell
& Wright, 1990)
American Sign Language signs(for deaf subjects) versus English words
(for hearing subjects) (Bavelier, Newport, Hall, Supalla, & Boutla, 2008;
Bellugi & Siple, 1974; Bellugi, Klima, & Siple, 1975; Boutla, Supalla,
Newport, & Bavelier,2004 ; Geraci, Gozzi, Papagno, & Cecchetto, 2008;
Krakow & Hanson, 1985)
Fingerspelled words (for deaf subjects) versus English words (for hearing
subjects) (Krakow & Hanson, 1985)
Various researchers have discussed the reasons for this deficit. Their hypotheses include the
longer articulation length of signs compared to speech (Wilson & Emmorey, 1997), the shorter
decay rate of visual/sign memory compared to echoic/speech-based memory (Boutla, Supalla,
Newport & Bavelier, 2004), and the formational complexity of signs versus speech (Geraci,
Gozzi, Papagno, & Cecchetto, 2008). Regardless of the theoretical viewpoint, deaf individuals
sequential based WM appears to be somewhat limited when compared to hearing individuals. A
recent review (Marschark & Wauters, 2008 ) has suggested that deaf children are less likely
than hearing children to utilize sequential processing strategies and this may account for at least
some of their linguistic WM deficit and language comprehension difficulties.
Processing speed.
Processing speed refers to the speed with which an individual can perform a cognitive
task such as recognizing a word or sign or comprehending a sentence. Speed of processing
deficits have been found to inhibit the oral and written language as well as the math ability of
hearing children (Fletcher, Lyon, Fuchs & Barnes, 2007; Mather, & Jaffe, 2002; Prifitera,
MEMORY SKILLS 7
Saklofske, &Weiss, 2005). Slow word recognition while reading has also been related to deficits
in reading fluency and comprehension (Kelly, 1993; Johns, 2009; Nagy, Anderson, Schommer,
Scott, & Stallman, 1989). Deaf students have been shown to have processing speed deficits
based on the results of the Processing Speed index subtests of the WISC-4 (Leutzinger, 2002;
Maller & Ferron 1997). Processing Speed subtests are also positively correlated with academic
achievement of deaf students (Braden, 1990; Kelly & Braden, 1990; Stewart, 1981).
According to Felser and Clahsen (2009) children and late-second language learners
usually exhibit slower language processing speed than mature native speakers. For children, this
slower processing speed is most likely due to their reduced attention and WM spans as compared
to adults. For non-native late second language learners, language processing is thought to be
cognitively more demanding than for native adults. While children seem to be able to use
monolingual adult-like processing routines from fairly early on in development, late-learners’
processing of some aspects of grammar appear to remain non-native-like even at higher
proficiency levels.
As learners experience a language, spoken or signed, one of the primary tasks is to
separate out the discrete symbols of the language from the flow of sound or sign for
comprehension or acquisition purposes (Felser & Clahsen, 2009; Hirsh-Pasek & Gollinkoff,
1996). Mayberry and Fischer (1989) have found that non-native signers are still struggling with
this task even in adulthood as compared to native signers who exhibit language processing skills
more indicative of automatic sign recognition. For non-native signers this bottleneck” in
processing has been related to deficits in the recall and comprehension of signing.
MEMORY SKILLS 8
Attention.
Attention is the cognitive process of focusing on one aspect of the immediate
environment and is of great importance in the function of WM (Engle, 2002). Attention as
measured subjectively by the Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity Comprehensive
Teacher Rating Scale, the Attention-Activity section of the ANSER, and the Conners' Parent
Rating Scale indicated that 14.1% of deaf children of deaf parents would be considered to have
attention deficits compared to 38.7% of deaf children of hearing parents (Kelly, et al, 1993).
Approximately 8-10 % of hearing children in the United States have been diagnosed with
attention deficits (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2010). Other subjective rating
scales have suggested no difference between deaf and hearing children in attention skills
(Meadow, 1976).
On empirical measures of attention, deaf children have been compared to hearing
children. These comparisons have revealed:
deficits (Altshuler, Deming, Vollenweider, Ranier, & Tendler,1976; Mitchell &
Quittner, 1996; Mykelbust & Brutten , 1953; Proksch & Bavalier, 2002; Parasnis,
Samar & Berent, 2003; Werner & Strauss, 1941)
superior ability (Larr, 1956; McKay, 1952).
Deaf individuals are better at attending to and processing information in peripheral vision than
hearing individuals (Chen, Zhang & Zhou, 2006; Loke & Song, 1991). The lack of hearing to
alert the deaf to the location of motion or animate objects in the environment may have fostered
this compensation. However, in a classroom where attention should be centered on the teacher or
interpreter attending to peripheral movement may be problematic (Dye, Hauser, & Bavelier,
2008). Sustaining and appropriately directing attention in the classroom does appear to be
MEMORY SKILLS 9
troublesome for deaf students. Matthews and Reich (1993) found that deaf high school students
attended to a classmate’s signing about 30% of the time when that classmate was communicating
with the teacher about class material. When the teacher was signing to the whole class the
students attended to the teacher 44% of the time. If the teacher addressed a particular student,
that student’s attention to the teacher increased to 50%. Marschark, et al., (2005) found similar
inattentiveness in college classrooms of deaf students.
Memory load.
Memory load is the cognitive complexity a task presents to an individual. For example,
the memory load inherent in comprehending a twelve word sentence is higher than that for
comprehending a three word sentence. As memory load increases performance often decreases
(Denh, 2008). One factor that increases memory load is the redundancy and juxtaposition of
similar words in a sentence. For hearing individuals such sentences are termed tongue-twisters
(“She sells seashells by the seashore”). Tongue-twisters increase task memory load.
Subsequently, comprehension is significantly less accurate for these sentence types as compared
to simple control sentences (“She buys her clothes at Old Navy.”) (Kennison, Sieck, & Briesch,
2003; McCutchen & Perfetti, 1982; Perfetti & McCutchen, 1982). Thus, formational similar
items employed in the dame utterance can increase memory load.
When processing sign language deaf adults have been shown to code items based on the
cherological (Stokoe, 1960) or sign-based formational features of the items (Bellugi, Klima &
Siple, 1975; Hamilton & Holzman; 1989; Hanson, 1982; Shand, 1982; Wilson & Emmorey,
1997, 1998). Deaf children have also shown evidence of cherological coding for signs
(Hamilton, 1984, 1985; Hirsh-Pasek & Treiman, 1982; Treiman & Hirsh-Pasek, 1983). Print, it
appears, can be coded phonologically (Hanson, 1990; Hanson & Lichtenstein, 1990) or
MEMORY SKILLS 10
cherologically (Krakow & Hanson, 1985; Shand & Klima, 1981; Wilson & Emmorey, 1997,
1998). For deaf individuals, Treiman and Hirsh-Pasek (1983) examined the comprehension of
“finger-fumbler” sentences (Kilma & Bellugi, 1979 ) in which signs for the printed words were
formationally similar. Results indicated that as task difficulty increased, reading comprehension
of single sentences decreased for less-proficient deaf readers. Comprehension was significantly
lower for sentences which contained words whose signs were formationally similar such as “I ate
apples at home yesterday. than for control sentences such as “I ate the bananas at work last
week.”. According to Treiman and Hirsh-Pasek, the underlined words have signs which are
considered visually similar. These words were culled from the data collected by Bellugi and
Siple (1974), Bellugi, Klima, and Siple (1975), and Klima and Bellugi (1979). Their similarity
lies in the fact that they are all produced in locations around the mouth and lower front side of
the face. EAT and HOME also share handshape while HOME and YESTERDAY share a similar
movement and location.
In the area of sequential recall, Rudner and Ronnberg (2008) have provided evidence that
deaf adults are similar to hearing adults in sequential recall of pictures when memory load
requirements are low. However as memory load increases sequential recall becomes more
difficult for deaf individuals sooner than for hearing individuals.
Memory Strengths and Research Questions for Language Learning and Education
Free recall.
For free recall or recalling a list in any order, memory span is equivalent in adult deaf
ASL signers and hearing English speakers for printed words (Hanson,1982, 1990) and ASL signs
and spoken words, respectively (Boutla, Supalla, Newport and Bavelier (2004). For children
MEMORY SKILLS 11
Liben (1979) has found free recall for line drawings to be similar for deaf and hearing subjects.
Similarly, there is no significant difference between the free recall of sequentially presented
shapes by deaf and hearing children (Todman & Seedhouse, 1994). Can this strength be useful
in academic learning where free recall ability is beneficial such as remembering the names of the
states or the bones in the body?
Visuospatial recall.
Visuospatial recall refers to the recall of items presented in some form of visual array
such as blocks on a table or objects in a grid. For sequential recall of nonlinguistic visuospatial
items, such as in the Corsi block test, deaf adults and children prove superior to hearing
individuals (Alamargot, Lambert, Thebault,& Dansac, 2007; Geraci, Gozzi, Papagno, &
Cecchetto, 2008; Logan, Mayberry, & Fletcher, 1996; Wilson, Bettger, Niculae, & Klima, 1997).
In the Corsi block test, the experimenter touches a static series of blocks randomly arranged on a
board and the subject must touch the blocks in the same sequential order. In a similar task, the
Knox Cube test which employs a static straight line of blocks, deaf children are also superior to
hearing children in sequential recall of this visuospatial array (Blair, 1957). Deaf children have
also shown equal sequential visuospatial recall ability to hearing children in the Simon game in
which a sequence of flashing colored lights arranged in a circle is recalled by touching them in
order of presentation (Tomlinson-Keasey & Smith-Winberry, 1990).
Also in the nonlinguistic visuospatial domain, Parasnis, Samar, Betger, and Sathe (1996)
utilizing the Revised Visual Retention Test (Benton,1974) found no significant difference
between hearing and deaf children in their ability to recall (by drawing) a series of geometric
figures presented via a static sequential pattern (a line of figures presented all at once). Utilizing
adult native deaf signers and the ReyOsterrieth Complex Figure Test, Hauser, Dye, Cohen, and
MEMORY SKILLS 12
Bavelier (2007) found no significant difference between hearing and deaf subjects on recall by
drawing of simple and complex geometric figures. The ReyOsterrieth Complex Figure Test
addresses spatial perception and visual memory. Similar results on this test have been found for
deaf children (Eldredge, 1984; Eldredge & Zhang, 1988; Parasnis & Kirk, 2004).
Deaf adolescent and adult subjects also have performed equal to hearing subjects on recall of
static sequential presentation of shapes (a line of shapes shown all at once) but less well than
the hearing subjects on temporal sequentially presented shapes (the shapes were presented one at
a time and disappeared before the next shape appeared) which required serial recall. Similar
results were found when digits (linguistic stimuli) were utilized as the recall items (Olsson &
Furth, 1966).
Also investigating linguistic items, Flaherty and Moran (2004) studied the sequential
recall of deaf and hearing college students who read phonologically-based English and Japanese
deaf and hearing college students familiar with reading kanji symbols (logographs) which are not
phonologically based. This study found deaf participants showed shorter sequential memory
spans than hearing participants for English words. However, sequential memory spans were
similar for deaf and hearing participants for words in kanji. Japanese deaf students reported using
a visual gestalt memory strategy, seeing the sequence as a whole rather than the sequential
strategy often reported by the English-reading deaf students. Similar results were revealed in a
study utilizing only hearing and deaf Japanese students (Flaherty & Moran, 2001).
Investigating free recall of visuospatially arranged linguistic items, Blair (1957) found
deaf children superior to hearing children in the free recall of everyday objects placed on a grid.
The children were shown fifteen items on a grid for twenty seconds. The items were then
removed and the children’s task was to place them back in their original location.
MEMORY SKILLS 13
It appears that deaf individuals strength appears to lie in the recall of information presented
in static visuospatial format. This appears to hold for both nonlinguistic and linguistic items.
Can educators devise presentation strategies which allow deaf students to take advantage of this
memory strength for the processing of sequential linguistic information, particularly English
print?
Imagery.
Imagery is the ability to create, maintain, and manipulate a visual image in WM. Enhanced
visuospatial abilities of deaf individuals compared to hearing individuals have been reported for
imagery (Blair, 1957; Emmorey & Kosslyn, 1995; Emmorey, Kosslyn, & Bellugi,1993; McKee,
1988) and mental rotation of visuospatial stimuli (Emmorey, Klima, &Hickcok, 1998; McKee,
1988). Can use of the deaf individuals enhanced imagery ability be utilized for increased
learning and academic achievement to either enhance WM or reduce the WM load presented by
a learning task?
Dual-encoding.
Dual-encoding refers to the individual’s use of both sign and speech codes when signs and
speech are presented simultaneously. This simultaneous presentation is called simultaneous
communication. Though often maligned in research literature, simultaneous communication of
lists of words was recalled better than sign-only and speech-only presentations by both hearing
and deaf signers. This effect was particularly strong for deaf signers (Hamilton & Holzman,
1989). Problematically, however, research has shown that skill in the use of simultaneous
communication is often erratic with elements of the syntax, grammar, and meaning of a message
being inconsistent. In the classroom, most teachers use a form of PSE that is neither a strict
coding of English nor ASL but contains features of both languages, along with speech, and
MEMORY SKILLS 14
consistently follows English word order (Akamatsu, Stewart & Mayer, 2004). Hearing teachers
using PSE often drop signs from signed sentences (Kluwin 1981; Luetke-Stahlman, 1988;
Marmor & Petitto 1979; Woodward and Allen, 1988). I n classroom signing Luetke-Stahlman
(1991) found that hearing teachers trying to represent English when signing were able to encode
the meaning of the target sentence about 71 percent of the time and omitted a sign or sign marker
over 50 percent of the time and also used wrong or invented signs. These same signers believed
they were accurately communicating via signs. Other researchers, however, found that if teachers
were given appropriate training and were committed to signing English, they could effectively
sign at a speech-to-sign ratio of greater than 90 percent. This was not the majority of hearing
signers, however (Mayer and Lowenbraun, 1990).
Research is needed in this area. Is recall and comprehension of simultaneous communication
superior to sign-only communication in the classroom during presentation of information more
complex than simple word lists? If so, can the utilization of simultaneous communication be
improved in general or should its use be targeted for controlled simple communication settings?
Memory Strengths when Strategy Employed
Phonological encoding.
Phonological encoding refers to speech-based/articulatory encoding (Dodd & Hermelin,
1977; Hanson, 1991). This forms the basis for the “functional equivalence hypothesis” as stated
by McQuarrie and Parrila (2009)
“The central claim of the functional equivalence hypothesis posits that visible speech
information (seen articulatory gesture) extracted from the speech signal by the deaf
learner is interpreted as a phonologically plausible signal by the brain (Campbell, 1987;
Dodd, 1976; Dodd & Hermelin, 1977). …On this basis, it has been further suggested that
MEMORY SKILLS 15
with the help of the visual information acquired through speechreading (Campbell, 1987;
Dodd, 1976; Dodd & Hermelin, 1977) and the articulatory feel of words that comes
through intensive speech training (Marschark & Harris, 1996), deaf children can develop
phonological representations of words. “
The use of a speech-based phonological code has been positively correlated with reading
comprehension in hearing children (Cain, 2006, de Jonge & de Jonge , 1996; Engle, Carullo, &
Collins, 1991; Engle, Kane, & Tuholski, 1999; Goswami & Bryant, 1990). Thus, phonological
encoding is currently a “hot” topic in deaf education, particularly in the area of reading (Allen, et
al., 2009; Paul, Wang, Trezek, & Luckner 2009; Mayberry, del Giudice, & Lieberman, 2011;
Wang, Trezek, Luckner & Paul, 2008).
It appears, though, that when reading, some deaf individuals employ phonological
encoding while others do not. Research indicates that deaf children are less likely than hearing
children to employ phonological coding in WM, reading, and spelling across a range of tasks
(Beech & Harris, 1997; Harris & Beech, 1998; Leybaert & Alegria, 1995; Merrills, Underwood,
& Wood, 1994; Nielsen & Luetke-Stahlman, 2002; Transler & Reitsma, 2005). When employed,
phonological or articulatory-based coding has been shown to facilitate sequential recall by deaf
adults (Kyle, 1981; Lichtenstein, 1998) and children (MacSweeney, 1998) and also been
positively correlated with reading comprehension ability of deaf individuals (Campbell &
Wright, 1988; Dyer, MacSweeney, Szczerbinski, Green, & Campbell 2003; Harris and
Beech,1998; Kyle & Harris, 2006, 2010; Lichtenstein, 1985, 1998; Perfetti & Sandak,2000;
Wang, Trezek, Luckner, & Paul, 2008). No positive relation has been found between phonemic
awareness (the ability to hear, identify, and manipulate phonemes) and reading ability of deaf
students (Harris & Beech, 1998; Kyle & Harris, 2006; Narr, 2008).
MEMORY SKILLS 16
Mayberry, del Giudice, & Lieberman (2011) in a meta-analysis of studies investigating
phonological encoding and reading in deaf students found that phonological encoding accounted
for only about 11% of the variance in reading ability while language ability accounted for 35%
of the variance. Not surprisingly, language does account for more variance in reading ability than
phonological encoding. The development of language is crucial for all aspects of a child’s life
and sign language is often the most effective tool for facilitating language acquisition for deaf
children. That does not negate the fact that phonological encoding does account for some
variance in reading ability and is an important area to consider.
Thus, the ability to code information phonologically is a WM strength which should be
considered when designing instruction. Phonological encoding does not rely on higher level
phonemic awareness for which hearing ability seems important. Rather phonological encoding
for deaf individuals appears to rely on whole word phonological/articulatory encoding that may
be enhanced via the development of speechreading which has been positively correlated with
reading ability of deaf children (Harris & Moreno, 2006; Kyle & Harris, 2006, 2010).
Can speechreading training which specifically targets the development of
phonological/articulatory coding enhance the sequential recall, and language and reading
comprehension of deaf students? Can speech articulation training enhance the quality and use of
phonological/articulatory encoding?
Rehearsal.
Rehearsal refers to the overt or covert repetition of items to be recalled or learned. For
deaf learners overt sign rehearsal has been shown to increase immediate sequential recall of :
printed words (Bonvillian, Rea, Orlansky, & Slade , 1987)
images (Bebko, 1984; Bebko & McKinnon, 1990)
MEMORY SKILLS 17
signed phrases (Weaver, Hamilton, Bruckman,& Starner, 2010).
It is important to note that deaf students do not spontaneously utilize rehearsal as early in
life as hearing students. Rehearsal appears in hearing students around age 7-8 as compared to
deaf signing students at age 10 or later (Bebko, 1979; Flavell, Chinsky, & Beach, 1966, Gill,
Klecan-Aker, Roberts, & Fredenburg, 2003). However, after instruction in overt rehearsal and
employment of this strategy deaf students have performed as well as hearing students in recall
tasks (Bebko, 1984; Belmont, Karchmer, and Pilkonis, 1976).
One study has reported evidence of 6- and 8-year old deaf children spontaneously using
both sign- and speech-based rehearsal during recall tasks for pictures, shapes, fingerspelling, and
print. Rehearsal, however, did not appear to enhance recall for these children (Liben & Drury,
1977). The emergence of rehearsal in deaf students appears to be directly related to language
experience, (Bebko & McKinnon, 1990) and more specifically, language proficiency and
automatized or automatic language processing. (Bebko & Metcalf-Haggart, 1997; Bebko, Bell,
Metcalfe-Haggert,& McKinnon, 1998).
Implications for Learning
Memory Deficits
Perhaps the most striking implication regarding the deaf individual’s deficiencies in WM
lies is the fact that they are all processes that are utilized during the comprehension and learning
of language. Attention is absolutely necessary as a first step in acquiring language data in the
environment. Processing speed must then be adequate to encode and manipulate this data.
Automatized recognition of signs is imperative so that the “bottleneck” described by Mayberry
and Fischer (1989) does not stress memory load causing processing difficulties. Finally, the
ability to maintain sequential linguistic information in WM is a key component of cognition,
MEMORY SKILLS 18
particularly during language parsing (McElree, Foraker,& Dyer, 2003; Sperber, Premack, &
Premack,1995 ). Willis and Gathercole (2001) have suggested that limited, less accurate
sequential WM ability may be responsible for slow acquisition of language in hearing children
and thus sequential memory skills are considered a crucial part of the language learning
mechanism for young children. The lack of sequential processing skills or failure to use a
sequential strategy during processing of linguistic information in WM may limit the deaf
individual’s ability to grasp syntactic order. Such a deficit can negatively affect language
development and subsequently comprehension of signed or printed material which negatively
impacts academic achievement.
With deficient WM abilities deaf children of hearing parents, in particular, are put in
double jeopardy for communicative and academic failure. Not only are they deprived of
language interaction (Goldin-Meadow & Mylander, 1990; Goldin-Meadow,1999; Gallaudet
Research Institute, 2008; Lederberg, 2006) that fosters communicative and academic growth
(and most likely WM capacity for language), they are attempting to process the relatively few
accessible linguistic interactions they are privy to with WM abilities that are sub-par compared to
hearing children who receive a wealth of linguistic input and interaction. The quantity and
quality of language interaction has also been related to language learning and educational
achievement of hearing children (Risley & Hart, 2002). Research has found strong predictive
relationships between language skills and reading ability which is a major component of
academic achievement for hearing children (Bowey & Patel, 1988; Dickinson, McCabe,
Anastasopoulos, Peisner-Feinberg, & Poe, 2003; Juel, Griffith, & Gough, 1986; Snow, Tabors, &
Dickinson, 2001) and deaf children (Harris & Moreno, 2004; Kyle & Harris, 2006; Mayberry,
del Giudice, & Lieberman, 2011; Padden & Hanson, 2000; Strong and Prinz, 1997). The
MEMORY SKILLS 19
synergistic relationship between language, WM, and reading is currently realized in deaf high
school students, as 50% read at the 4
th
grade level or below upon graduation(Gallaudet Research
Institute, 1996; Traxler, 2000) and 30% leave high school functionally illiterate (Marschark,
1997; Marschark, Lang, & Albertini, 2002). The academic achievement of deaf students has
remained at these levels for approximately thirty years (Qi & Mitchell, 2007) regardless of the
educational or language policy of the day. Language delay and educational underachievement of
deaf individuals may then be attributed to at least two factors; lack of accessible linguistic
interaction with skilled signers and subsequently deficient WM skills to assist during language
and academic learning.
Memory Strengths
The WM strengths of deaf individuals in the areas of free recall, imagery, visuospatial
recall, dual encoding, phonological encoding, and rehearsal all have implications for improving
the design and delivery of instruction. The deaf individual’s strengths can be utilized and
deficiencies remediated or compensated for so that communication and academic achievement
can be enhanced. The WM strengths, just listed, are applied in the instructional design of the
WM interventions described below in order to enhance processing skills and subsequent
learning. Other strategies may also be useful and empirical validation is necessary in all cases.
Applications for learning
WM Interventions
Gerneral definitions.
Now that the WM deficiencies and strengths of deaf learners and the subsequent
implications have been described how can these deficiencies be remediated or strengths utilized
to enhance learning? One way is through WM interventions. Feifer and DeFina (2000) have
MEMORY SKILLS 20
suggested that memory intervention is most successful during early childhood and early
elementary years due to brain maturation. Change is more difficult once neural structures are
established and myelination is complete. However several studies have shown that children age
7-15 can benefit from WM intervention (Comblain, 1994; McNamara & Scott, 2001; Minear &
Shah, 2006).
Denh (2008) describes interventions for WM as either compensatory or remedial.
Compensatory methods typically involve training in memory strategies and may also include
various external aids and methods for bypassing the deficient processes and reducing task
demands. Remedial methods generally address the individual’s memory deficits to enhance
them. The research literature is mixed in its findings regarding the effectiveness of remedial
interventions. Lee and Riccio (2005) found remedial intervention ineffective but others
(Comblain, 1994; Holmes, Gathercole & Dunning, 2009; Klingberg, Forssberg, & Westerberg
2002; Klingberg et al., 2005; Mezcappa & Buckner, 2010) have described successful remedial
interventions. A combined intervention approach utilizing both compensatory and remedial
techniques has been shown to be most successful (Denh, 2008).
Interventions can also be either domain specific or domain general. Domain specific
skills are those involving specific areas of knowledge such as language skills or math facts.
Domain general skills focus on higher order more abstract cognitive skills such as WM capacity
(Roberts, 2007). Remedial and compensatory interventions addressing these two domains will
now be described. This list is not exhaustive as other activities can also serve to address WM
and enhance language learning and academic achievement.
Specific interventions.
Preschool years- birth to kindergarten.
MEMORY SKILLS 21
As has been stated in many research articles in the area of deafness early exposure to
accessible language is imperative. This often means sign language. Interaction with fluent
signers allows the child to develop the language and processing skills needed to achieve
academically. As a general rule of thumb for interacting with young children and beginning
signers, adults should sign slowly, clearly, and utilize short sentences so as not to overload the
child’s memory during processing. This is a strategy used by parents of young hearing children
(Snow, 1977), teachers of children learning English as a second language (Kottler, Kottler, &
Street, 2007) and is suggested for teachers of children with WM deficits (Gathercole & Alloway,
2008).
An environment in which the child is surrounded by fluent signers is often not available
to most deaf children, however (Goldin-Meadow & Mylander; 1990, Goldin-Meadow,1999;
Gallaudet Research Institute, 2008; Lederberg, 2006). As a substitute, signed video and games
may be tools that can help enhance the child’s facility with vocabulary, automatic sign
recognition, and hence WM. It should be stated, however, that signed video and games can not
be seen as an equal substitute for interaction with fluent signers.
During the sign presentation in the video or game it is probably best to have a still image
behind the signer as deaf individuals have been shown to attend to peripheral distractors (Chen,
Zhang & Zhou, 2006; Loke & Song, 1991) rather than the central information which in this case
is the signer. If the content of the media contains action that the signer is describing it may be
best to utilize a sequential presentation in which the signer is followed by the action, again for
attention reasons. The efficacy of signed videos and games and the particular presentation
formats that facilitate language processing and development are open research questions which
MEMORY SKILLS 22
merit investigation. Many signed videos and games are available on commercial DVDs and for
free at the “Electric Language Factory” (ELF, www.cats.gatech.edu/cats/ELF/index.htm).
Rehearsal can be utilized with young deaf children to improve sequential WM skills and
subsequently language processing skills by reciting nursery rhymes, singing songs, and
performing action chants. This is a common practice in many homes and preschools for hearing
children and may serve an unintended purpose of developing sequential WM for language. “Jack
be nimble” and “The wheels on the bus” are examples of English rhymes and songs,
respectively. Action chants are simple rhymes that are accompanied by physical actions as
opposed to simply saying the rhyme. “Ring around the rosie” is an English action chant in which
children hold hands and walk in a circle reciting the chant and then “all fall down”. An ASL
action chant which follows the format of an ASL number story could be “ONE, TWO
SQUIRRELS HOP-AROUND” after which the persons reciting the chant hop around the room.
Other examples of sign language nursery rhymes, songs, and chants are available on YouTube,
sign2me.com and in Hamilton (1987, 1988). As was noted earlier deaf children are similar to
hearing children in recall of sequential information when trained to rehearse. The rote recitation
inherent in producing signs of rhymes, songs, and action chants may aid in the development of
sign language and WM skills, particularly rehearsal. It is suggested that pictures, animations, or
physical responses such as hopping, as in the action chant above, accompany production of these
verses to allow for better understanding of the signing.
Completing daily household tasks can further assist the child in sequential WM
development. The adult can start by asking the child to do a single task such as “Get your shirt.”
And then progress to two tasks such as “Get your dirty clothes and put them in the wash” and
then to three or more tasks which should be done sequentially. The use of the ASL mechanism
MEMORY SKILLS 23
for referencing items of a list on the non-dominant hand (Baker-Shenk & Cokely, 1991) may
facilitate recall for the child as this provides a visuospatial reference for each item. Adults could
help the child rehearse the tasks to be done as both use the non-dominant hand placeholders.
Empirical validation of this ASL mechanism as a memory support tool is needed.
For free recall, the parent can tell the child what items are needed in the store during a
shopping trip and then have the child lead the search to find them. For young children who
cannot find items in a store yet the parent can simply tell the child an item or two they need as
they go through the store and when they find the item repeat the name of the item. Parents can
increase the number of items as the child becomes more adept with language. For other activities
that address WM with young children see Gibson, (2003).
School years- grade 1-12.
Regardless of students’ language background and WM ability schools are mandated to
teach academic content. For that reason, this paper will discuss techniques to address WM skills
within the domain specific areas of language arts, mathematics, and content area subjects as well
as techniques for managing WM load through instructional design.
Language art- Language comprehension.
The drag-and-drop feature available in PowerPoint provides an easy-to-use tool for
teachers to develop activities that focus on sequential memory during sentence comprehension.
Figure 1 shows a slide that can be used for such an activity. Many of the images are animated to
represent the action of the verb as clearly as possible. These and similar images are available at
www.animfactory.com
MEMORY SKILLS 24
Figure 1. Drag-and-drop slide
Using the slide in figure 1, the teacher can present a sentence in sign and a student can drag
images on to the white area to create a picture that represents the meaning of the sentence. The
student must maintain the sequential order of the sentence in WM long enough to comprehend it
and then manipulate the images to create the picture. It is important to use sentences in which the
subject and object are interchangeable. This forces the students to use sequential information to
correctly comprehend the sentence. Short sentences such as “The girl is scolding the man.” and
“The man is scolding the girl” are just two examples. Longer sentences such as “The mouse is
looking at the fat man” and “The man with the popcorn and drink is watching the girl who is
crying.” can also be used with this same slide.
MEMORY SKILLS 25
It is also important to use absurd or silly sentences that require sequential processing in
order to be understood correctly. The students must follow the word order of the sentence to
correctly comprehend the sentence even when the result produces an image with a low
probability of actually occurring in real life. A sentence such as “The cat is scolding the girl” can
be made using the images in figure 1. Until about age 4 years 6 months, hearing children will
often comprehend such sentences by using an “event probability” strategy (i.e., make sense of
the sentence regardless of word order), in this case producing a picture showing the girl with the
cake scolding the cat near the spilt milk. Evidence for this behavior has been found not only for
English speaking children (Stroehner & Nelson, 1974) but also for speakers of Italian (Bates,
1976; Bates, MacWhinney, Caselli, Devescovi, Natale, & Venza, 1984; Duranti & Ochs, 1979),
French (Sinclair & Bronckart, 1972), Spanish (Reyes, 2003), and German (Lindner, 2003). Data
collected by the author for ASL indicates non-native signers, hearing and deaf, tend to use “event
probability” during language processing, often to a greater extent than hearing children.
Also of interest in this area is the work of Treiman and Hirsh-Pasek (1983). Their
research indicated that sentences which contained signs which were visually similar were more
difficult to comprehend than sentences with visually dissimilar signs due to the WM load
involved in each (see discussion above). When constructing sentences for a comprehension task
teachers should be aware of this phenomenen so they can either avoid such sentences or include
such sentences depending on the students and goal of the lesson.
Language arts- Reading.
Reading English print is primarily a sequential WM processing task as English utilizes a
rather strict adherence to word order to communicate meaning. For instructional purposes, print
can actually serve to reduce WM load inherent in the sequential presentation of signs which
MEMORY SKILLS 26
appear and then are gone. Print provides a static visuaospatial sequential stimuli and allows the
teacher to visually reference key words or phrases by simply pointing to them. Thus, the
compensatory elements afforded by print and employed by the teacher may help reduce the WM
load inherent in processing sequential language for non-native signers. All words used in such an
activity must be automatized or the added memory load of encountering unknown words will
negate the advantage gained from the static sequential presentation.
The drag-and-drop task described above can also be utilized for reading instruction and will
allow for imagery to be used by the teacher and students to aid in comprehension. Rather than
single sentences, a story could be used as the content for the activity. For illustration purposes, a
reading activity addressing pronominal reference will be described. Using figure 1, the short
story presented could be “The man scolded the cat. He was angry. She spilled the milk”. After
creating a picture to show the meaning of the first sentence the images created can be used to
support further comprehension of the pronominal reference in the other two sentences. The
teacher can refer to the image created on the screen to show pronoun reference. A similar
sentence sequence could then be presented substituting different nouns and asking the students to
imagine what the scene would look like and then draw it, drag-and-drop images, or answer
questions about the new sentence sequence to indicate comprehension. Pictures and text from
storybooks, guided readers, or chapter books could also be used. A commercially available
program, the Lindamood-Bell Learning Process program, has been shown to improve reading
scores of hearing students by teaching the students to use visualization and imagery (Sadoski &
Willson, 2006). This may be a useful program for deaf students.
Processing speed is very important during reading (Legols & Perfetti, 1978; Perfetti &
Lesgold, 1978) and is best represented by the term automaticity, the instantaneous recognition of
MEMORY SKILLS 27
words. Grushkin (1998) has suggested that automatic word recognition can alleviate memory
load during the act of reading comprehension. Conversely, struggling to recognize words when
reading causes fewer WM resources to be available during reading comprehension (Denh, 2008).
Simple repetition activities to foster overlearning of printed words can help the learner attain
automaticity.
Reading words or sentences presented for a short period of time can assist in developing
speed of processing. This can be done in the classroom via PowerPoint presentation with the
word, sentence, or short paragraph presented on a slide which is set to transition to a blank slide
via the timer feature in PowerPoint. The use of tachistiscope programs may also be useful. There
are several free programs available on the internet such as RAM4
(http://www.slu.edu/colleges/AS/languages/classical/ram/ram.html). A low tech solution is to
simply present the target word(s) on a white board and then cover or erase them after a short
time.
Captioned video is also useful for building processing speed and sequential WM as the
caption presentation is time-limited and sequential. The teacher can pause the video immediately
after the presentation of the caption and ask the students what the caption said and questions
about the caption. The caption presentation also can build focused attention as the student must
ignore the activity on the screen and focus attention on the captions. Without automatic word
recognition, proficient processing speed, sufficient sequential WM, and the ability to focus
attention on the caption and not the peripheral action it would seem that captioned video is non-
beneficial to the viewer.
The lack of knowledge of English printed words also affects reading greatly. When
readers know less than 90% of the words in a passage comprehension drops to 50% or less
MEMORY SKILLS 28
(Johns, 2009). This is particularly true for deaf readers (Albertini & Mayer, 2011; Davey &
King, 1990; LaSasso & Davey, 1987; Paul, 1996; Paul & Gustafson, 1991; Paul & O'Rourke,
1988). During the reading of any print material, online or off, the English-ASL dictionary
SMARTSign-Dictionary (www.cats.gatech.edu) provides a tool for quickly finding a sign or
signs for a word and also reduces the memory load inherent in mentally searching for signs for
unknown or non-automatized words. Students can simply type the English word into the
SMARTSign-Dictionary and then see the sign(s) for that word. Often picture support is provided
to enhance the learning of the word-sign pair by providing imagery for the concept represented.
This is especially important for young readers who are new to signs and may be encountering the
sign for the first time via exposure to the English word in a book. “Google images” also provides
a powerful tool which allows users to enter words and search for images. “Google images”
essentially functions as a picture dictionary.
Online electronic dictionary use is common among adult second language learners. Lan
(2005) reports that over 70% of the interviewed students at Hong Kong Polytechnic University
who were learning English as a second language were online dictionary users. Use of an
electronic dictionary such as the SMARTSign-Dictionary may assist deaf readers during the
reading process. It can be used on desktop computers or laptop computers, tablet computers, and
cell phones for mobility.
As discussed earlier language ability accounts for a large part of reading ability. Viewing
the reading process as a whole, language skills, vocabulary knowledge, reading fluency as
evidenced by the automatized recognition of words, and general world knowledge also make
major contributions to reading comprehension (Denh, 2008). Phonological encoding is also
important. To develop phonological encoding, activities focusing on speechreading (Kyle &
MEMORY SKILLS 29
Harris, 2006, 2010) and articulation (McQuarrie & Parrila, 2009) can be done. It seems
important to relate speechread and spoken articulated words directly to known printed words in
order to have an effect on reading (Marschark & Harris, 1996).
In the classroom, speaking information during highly contextualized routine situations
can address phonological encoding via speechreading. If it is time for lunch and the teacher has
daily signed “Time for lunch” this information can be presented via signs then speech and finally
through speech alone as the students become familiar with the situation. It will be important to
ask the students what was said and then also quickly present the print for the spoken words to
make the speechreading-print connection. Asking the students to say the phrase will also build
the phonological/articulatory representation for the target phrase. It is likely that the
representation the student produces does not need to match a “perfect English” representation of
the words. The representation should however be different than the representation of other words
or phrases. This will allow the student to utilize an internally consistent phonological/articulatory
code which is of benefit in WM. If the students codes all words with the same articulatory code
(e.g., buh) it seems less likely that such a code will be beneficial. Speech therapists may make a
significant contribution to literacy development by including speachreading and articulation
activities related to print in their work with students. Research is needed to determine the
empirical validity of this hypothesis.
Language arts: Writing.
Building English schema via visuospatial scaffolding has proven successful in helping
deaf students develop basic English writing skills (Hamilton & Jones, 1989). According to Chi,
Glaser, and Rees (1982) a schema categorizes elements of information according to the manner
in which they will be used. Schemas are examples of sophisticated rules and are stored in long
MEMORY SKILLS 30
term memory. It is important for schemas to become automatized so they can be used quickly
and effortlessly. Practice with the schema help them become automatized (Paas, 1992). Learners
who have automated schema have more WM capacity available to use the schema to solve more
sophisticated problems (Sweller, 1988).
Denh (2008) describes scaffolding as a strategy that can enhance WM. Scaffolding provides
the learner with initial support for the learning task and gradually removes the support while
maintaining a low-error environment for the student. As students show success, the support is
removed until the student is performing the task correctly without the scaffolding. A tool that
provides scaffolding for building schema for writing basic English sentences, “Simple Sentence
Lab” (SSL), contains activities that are both compensatory and remedial in nature. Teachers can
utilize any subject matter in SSL simply by typing sentences into the program. The sentences
could be about a field trip, storybook, news event, or academic content from science or social
studies. As few as five and as many as fifteen sentences can be entered. These limits are
established to manage WM load by not overloading it. Long stories can be broken into chapters
and content information can be broken into multiple units if necessary. Over a dozen activities
which address sequential memory, sentence production, spelling, chunking of English phrases,
rehearsal, and schema building for written English are available and provided in a suggested
sequence that initially provides supportive scaffolding and then progressively removes it. Both
computer-based and paper-and-pencil tasks are utilized.
MEMORY SKILLS 31
Figure 2. Simple Sentence Lab screen
Figure 2 illustrates the schema and visual scaffolding provided for sentences entered into
the SSL program. Students can create a story, summarize content, or answer teacher questions
using this schema. As shown in figure 2, the student has already typed in the subject noun phrase
of a sentence and is ready to type the verb phrase. When that is done correctly the grid and text
entry box move to the third column and the student enters the final phrase. The scaffolding
support provided by the columns and sliding grid is faded away as students are successful. The
English sentences produced will be syntactically and grammatically correct due to the responses
allowed in each column. The visual schema and procedure of SSL allows only correct syntactic
sentences and a built-in grammar only allows grammatically correct responses. For example, the
student can type “Rattle snakes eat mice.” but not “Rattle snakes eats mice. As shown in Figure
MEMORY SKILLS 32
2 “eats” is grayed-out and SSL’s artificial intelligence will not allow it as an acceptable verb due
to the subject-verb agreement necessary with “Rattle snakes”. The semantic accuracy of the
students responses must be evaluated by the teacher. SSL would allow the grammatically
correct sentence “King snakes can’t kill mice.” when actually the opposite is true. This allows
teachers to see what students understand about the target content while providing scaffold-
supported practice for writing English.
SSL provides a grammatically “errorless” environment for learning English which is
important for students with WM deficits (Gathercole, Lamont, & Alloway, 2006). Compensatory
support via a static visuospatial organization of English utilizes the deaf individual’s strength in
sequential recall of static visuospatial items (the SSL grid) and also lessens sequential WM load
as well as organizing English words via grammatically-based chunking. Chunking is the
grouping of to-be-remembered items into meaningful rule-governed units. Verbal sequential
WM capacity expands as chunks are formed by the items to be managed in WM (Denh, 2008).
This is particularly useful in language processing as chunking allows an increase of nearly
threefold in memory span of native speakers when sentences rather than unrelated words are to
be recalled (Baddely, 2003, Case, 1977). Language chunks appear to be based on the rule
governed constituents of the particular language known by the individual such as noun-phrases
and verb-phrases (Case, 1977). Thus, chunking is a very important aspect of maintaining and
manipulating linguistic items in WM. Providing external visual aids that show the students
English chunks as in figure 2 may help them to create chunks corresponding to phrases or
clauses, thereby creating more manageable units of information (Montgomery, 2003). SSL is
free and available at www.cats.gatech.edu/cats/CatSoft/SSL.htm .
Language arts: Vocabulary and spelling.
MEMORY SKILLS 33
In this paper, learning vocabulary will refer to learning the meaning of unknown words or
signs. Once a word or sign is part of the students known vocabulary the spelling of the printed
English word can be learned. The student may be simultaneously learning vocabulary and the
spelling of that vocabulary in school.
Learning vocabulary in a classroom can be enhanced by utilizing visual imagery. Studies
indicate that the imageability of a word is a key factor in determining its ease of acquisition
(Gillett, Gleitman, Gleitman, & Lederer, 1999; Ma, Golinkoff, Hirsh-Pasek, McDonough, &
Tardif, 2009). Visual imagery has been shown to be particularly useful for students with deficits
in verbal WM when they possess a strong visuospatial WM. Mnemonic strategies in which a
verbal utterance, in this case the meaning of a word or sign, is related to a visual image have
been successfully used for many years to assist in recall and learning (Eslinger, 2002; Levin,
1993; Mastropieri & Scruggs, 1998; Pavio & Caspo, 1969). It is most effective when the image
created is unique, funny, or bizarre (Ritchie & Karge, 1996). As mentioned earlier, the
Lindamood-Bell Learning Process program focuses specifically on the use of imagery to enhance
reading and its tenets and procedures could be employed during vocabulary learning. For deaf
students providing an image along with a sign(s) for vocabulary would appear to be helpful.
Combining visualization of the meaning of the verbal string with rehearsal has been found to be
more effective than rehearsal alone (Clark & Klecan-Aker, 1992).
Learning the spelling of words (that is the sequence of letters in a word) generally
involves the use of rehearsal. For increasing sequential WM skills, rehearsal has been found to
be a useful strategy (Comblain, 1994; Minear & Shah, 2006) and facilitates information storage
in long-term memory (Denh, 2008). Training deaf students in the use of rehearsal at an early age
can pay benefits immediately and in the future.
MEMORY SKILLS 34
Chunking can be also used to reduce memory load for deaf students as they learn
vocabulary which will be used during reading or writing by taking advantage of the single sign
ASL representation of English phrases such as “look up”, “jump over”, and “get on”. When these
printed phrases are recognized as a chunk, sequential memory span has fewer items to maintain
thus reducing memory load. Teaching the student these English phrases as is done in the
Fairview method (Schimmel & Edwards, 2003; Schimmel, Edwards, & Prickett, 1999) may
prove beneficial.
For classroom vocabulary or spelling instruction the use of simultaneous communication
may be beneficial. Deaf individuals have been shown to recall simultaneous communicated lists
significantly better than lists that were presented only in sign (Hamilton & Holzman, 1989). By
utilizing simultaneous communication for controlled, structured classroom presentation of
vocabulary and spelling the memory enhancement of this dual code may be realized while
eliminating the detrimental aspects of simultaneous communication such as dropped signs and
faulty syntax. Drasgow and Paul (1995) suggest that the processing requirements for producing
simultaneous sign and speech causes signers to delete or incorrectly code signs due to WM
overload. By limiting simultaneous communication to short bits of information during
vocabulary and spelling instruction WM overload may be eliminated for the teachers while the
simultaneous signal enhances recall for the students. This suggestion requires empirical
validation.
Math.
Visual imagery has been shown to be particularly valuable in learning mathematics for
hearing students (McLean & Hitch, 1999) and deaf students (Blatto-Vallee, 2007; Lang &
Pagliaro, 2010). Nunes, (2006) has described visual displays that may enhance mathematics
MEMORY SKILLS 35
learning by deaf students. These provide scaffolding to support number recognition for early
math facts and for more advanced math processes such as word problems.
Static sequential visuospatial presentation of math facts (e.g., 2+4=6) provides students with
an information format that allows for use of their WM strength in this area and may foster more
efficient learning. Providing math fact tables for study may also assist students by providing a
visuospatial schema of the to-be-learned facts. This scaffolding can be faded away as it is
internalized and students automatize the math facts.
To increase processing speed and automaticity as well as attention the teacher can use a
timed PowerPoint presentation to flash a math fact or simply write the math fact on the
whiteboard and then erase it. After the math fact is removed from view students can write it on
paper in a race type of format which will also encourage an increase in processing speed.
Dictating math problems to students may also aid the development of WM and math skills.
This is a remedial intervention designed to “exercise” and increase WM as opposed to the
compensatory strategy described earlier utilizing static sequential visuaspatial presentation. The
students can write the dictated problem on paper and then solve it. If needed, the words used in
the dictation can be randomly displayed to eliminate the WM load caused by unknown spellings.
Alternatively, the words could be arranged in a visual schema similar to the one shown earlier in
Figure 2. This schema adds scaffolding for recalling the dictation using English syntax.
By writing the problem on paper WM load is reduced during problem solution and the
teacher also can see how much of the problem the students actually recalled. To exercise and
enhance WM load capabilities the teacher can require the students to solve the problem without
writing it on paper after they are successful in the writing task. This is an example of scaffolding
being removed.
MEMORY SKILLS 36
Using the ASL grammatical feature in which objects are located in space and then referred to
may also be an effective means of reducing working memory load. This type of presentation
would also allow deaf students to employ imagery to “see” the math problem and allow the
teacher to manually manipulate the invisible items to explain the necessary mathematical
process. For example, the word problem “Jack has 3 dogs. Jill has two dogs. How many dogs do
they have all together?” could be signed by placing Jack’s 3 dogs in a location in the left of the
signing space, Jill’s 2 dogs in a location to the right and holding the signs for 3 and 2 in the
respective locations. The signs can then be brought together in a middle location showing that 3
and 2 combine.
Memory load can also be reduced during math activities by providing calculators for students
who have not automatized math facts. This would be useful in learning the process of balancing
a checkbook or planning a budget. For higher order math processes such as those in geometry,
trigonometry, or calculus automatized math facts are imperative so that WM can focus on the
math processes involved in these subject areas and not be overloaded by deficient computational
knowledge.
Content areas: Science, Social Studies.
Deaf individuals have shown equal free recall abilities as compared to hearing individuals
across several tasks. Two particular types of learning tasks, labeling tasks such as labeling the
fifty states on a map, and categorizing tasks such as categorizing animals versus plants lend
themselves to free recall. These types of activities accompanied by rehearsal practice can
enhance the learning of content which is not sequentially bound.
Rehearsal can be valuable in the area of learning factual information in content areas. Using
repetition for this important information can facilitate long term storage of these facts (Denh,
MEMORY SKILLS 37
2008) and improve the automaticity of their access for higher level cognitive processes such as
understanding chemical bonding between atoms or the principles of a democratic government.
By utilizing the consistent visuospatial schema for English as provided by SSL (see figure 2)
with a variety of content, the content information may be learned more efficiently and English
language skills may increase (Hamilton & Jones, 1989). Variability of practice materials results
in beneficial effects on transfer of learning (Cormier and Hagman, 1987; Jelsma, van
Merrienboer, and Bijlstra, 1990; Singley and Anderson,1989). Thus, variability (different subject
area content) over the problem situation (learning the content and producing English sentences)
is expected to encourage learners to develop efficient schemas for the target information because
it increases the probability that similar features can be identified and that relevant features can be
distinguished from irrelevant ones. Consistent use of a tool such as SSL is imperative if it is to be
successful in building schema.
Rudner and Ronberg (2008) suggest that a presentation style that places less emphasis on
the temporal order of information may facilitate recall performance for deaf subjects. The use of
visuospatial tools such as flowcharts, boxes, or diagrams fit this presentation style. Such tools
reduce memory load (Grushkin, 1998) and subsequently enhance recall and learning. O’Donnell
and Adenwalla (1991) compared the use of visually diagrammed information maps and texts by
deaf undergraduate biology students. These students scored higher on recall and multiple choice
comprehension tasks when using the visually mapped information for learning purposes.
“Thinking maps” (Hyerle & Yeager, 2007, www.thinkingmaps.com) take advantage of
the visuospatial abilities of deaf students for static presentations of information. These are
graphic organizers with different visual structures that are designed to consistently represent the
same type of relationships between information thus building a schema for the content. Such
MEMORY SKILLS 38
graphic organizers are especially powerful when the students create the organization of the visual
schema themselves (Davies, 1980). “Inspiration”, “FreeMind”, and “XMind” are computer
programs that allow for quick free-form construction of visual graphic organizers. The last two
listed are free and available online. Luckner, Bowen, and Carter (2001) have also described
visual displays for reducing WM load for deaf students.
General concerns for all instruction.
Attention.
Attention is extremely important during WM processing (Engle, 2002). If a student is not
attending, little information can be acquired or comprehended. An attention strength of deaf
individuals may also serve to be a problem during language processing. Deaf individuals are
highly attuned to information in peripheral vision. Thus, movement on either side of the student
or teacher can be distracting. To develop attention to the signer and not peripheral movement a
teacher could give directions while standing near a computer or television screen that displays
some type of movement. Students then follow the directions. Start with short simple directions
and progress to longer directions. It may be useful to explain to the students the purpose of the
activity in order to enlist a metacognitive strategy that encourages the students to make a
concerted effort to attend to the signer. Also, by adding a distractor such as a moving image to a
slide of information being presented focused attention can also be addressed. However, be aware
that adding such distractors may cause even greater loss of attention to the teacher than the 50%
described by Matthews and Reich (1993). Research into the use of such attention building
activities is needed.
Memory load.
MEMORY SKILLS 39
The classroom is notorious for overloading the memory abilities of all students on a daily
basis (Denh, 2008). Assessing the WM load of a task and adjusting it as necessary is important
for facilitating student success. Some general principles for reducing WM load are:
Language processing, particularly of long utterances, may overload WM.
Comprehension of verbal material will be enhanced by using language that is simple,
structured, and redundant.
Multitasking by students places undo strain on attention and hence WM. Focus on
one activity at a time.
Students with WM deficits can learn if they have ample exposure to material while
the demands on WM are minimal.
Allow students time to process new information. More learning occurs when students
are given time to rehearse the information and apply memory strategies.
Repetition or practice of a task is very important.
External support aids such as visual cues, checklists, and prompts will reduce WM
load.
Provide learners with graduated learning support (scaffolding) until the support is no
longer needed. Gathercole, Lamont, and Alloway (2006) have suggested that
“errorless learning”, where errors are prevented or minimized, is much more effective
for individuals with WM deficits than “errorful learning”, essentially learning by trial
and error. Several studies have shown this to be the case (Baddeley & Wilson, 1994;
Clare, Wilson, Carter, Roth & Hodges, 2002; Hamilton & Jones 1989). If a child has
a WM deficit it is extremely important to minimize task failure due to WM load.
MEMORY SKILLS 40
American Sign Language may also help reduce WM load. Geraci, Gozzi, Papagno, and
Cecchetto (2008) have suggested that ASL grammar has evolved overtime to utilize the
enhanced visuospatial memory abilities of deaf individuals and downplay the deficits. Such a
development seems only natural. Research should address how the use of ASL (for reducing
WM load) and simultaneous communication (for providing an enhanced signal which is recalled
better than sign alone) can be best utilized for communication and instruction.
WM activities in the classroom.
Denh (2008) suggests the following tenets for addressing WM in the classroom. WM
activities should:
be brief and focused on one strategy.
be spaced, with two or three per week over a long period of time.
provide plenty of practice that allows the child to utilize the target strategy and
encourage the child to attribute his/her success to the strategy.
provide multiple sessions so that ultimately the strategy is overlearned.
provide positive reinforcement for successful use of the strategy.
include teaching a child when and how to use a strategy so the child can apply this
metacognitive knowledge as necessary.
match the needs of the learner and be adaptive. As the child’s skills increase so
should the task difficulty (Holmes, Gathercole & Dunning, 2009; Klingberg et al.,
2005; Klingberg, Forssberg, & Westerberg 2002)
Utilizing these tenets and enhancing WM can facilitate language learning and academic
achievement of deaf students
MEMORY SKILLS 41
Conclusion
This paper has reviewed literature on the memory skills of deaf learners and
described activities which take advantage of the deaf individual’s memory strengths to
facilitate learning and reduce memory load or to enhance deficit memory skills which are
important to learning. The deaf learner’s memory deficiencies include sequential
memory, processing speed, attention, and memory load. Strengths include free recall,
visuospatial recall, imagery and dual encoding. When utilized, phonological encoding
and rehearsal emerge as strategies which enhance recall. These strategies are not
spontaneously used by all deaf individuals, however, and may be prime candidates for
instruction.
Instructional design and classroom practices can utilize deaf learners’ memory
strengths, compensate for weaknesses, and attempt to remediate basic information
processing skills so that linguistic competence and academic achievement can be
increased. Activities that can address these areas have been described above. Not all
suggested activities have been empirically validated as the importance of WM in
education is just now being realized. The specific questions below should be investigated.
Can visuospatial organization facilitate recall and learning of sequential linguistic
information such as English syntax?
Can deaf individuals utilize imagery for increased learning and academic
achievement?
Can viewing signed video and playing sign-enhanced video games enhance
language learning?
MEMORY SKILLS 42
Due to the visual constraint of being able to attend to one item at a time (Wolfe,
2000) what particular presentation formats for media can best facilitate language
comprehension and learning?
Can attention to the signer, rather than to peripheral distractors in a classroom, be
trained so it exceeds the current 50% benchmark?
Can speechreading training which specifically targets development of
phonological/articulatory coding enhance the sequential recall, and language and
reading comprehension of deaf students?
Can speech articulation training facilitate phonololgical/articulatory coding to
enhance recall regardless of vocal intelligibility?
Is recall and comprehension of simultaneous communication superior to sign-only
communication in the classroom during presentation of information more
complex than simple word lists? If so, can the utilization of simultaneous
communication be improved? Is simultaneous communication best utilized only
in limited presentation/communication instances as opposed to open-ended
communication?
Can use of ASL spatially established referents and manipulation of these referents
facilitate math problem solving?
How can the use of ASL (for reducing WM load) and simultaneous
communication (for providing an enhanced signal which is recalled better than
sign alone) be best utilized for communication and instruction?
MEMORY SKILLS 43
Finally, instructional practice can adopt the suggestions of Denh (2008) and others for
limiting working memory load and creating error-free learning environments to enhance
learning. For example, the use of visual schemas and scaffolding hold great promise for deaf
education due to their ability to reduce memory load and call into play the visuosapatial WM
strengths of deaf students. This paper has provided a starting point for raising the awareness of
educators of deaf students on the important issue of WM. It has also detailed suggestions for
areas of future research investigating the utility of specific WM activities. The field of WM and
its application to education provide new and exciting possibilities for enhancing language
learning and academic achievement of deaf students
MEMORY SKILLS 44
References
Akamatsu, C. T., Stewart, D., & Mayer, C. (2004). Is it time to look beyond teachers’
signing? In G. Leigh (Ed.), Educating deaf students (pp. 4054). Washington, DC:
Gallaudet University Press.
Alamargot, D., Lambert, E., Thebault, C., & Dansac, C. (2007). Text
composition by deaf and hearing middle-school students: The role of working memory.
Reading and Writing, 20, 333-360.
Allen, T.E., Clark, M.D., del Giudice, A., Koo, D., Lieberman, A., Mayberry, R, & Miller, P.
(2009). Phonology and Reading: A Response to Wang, Trezek, Luckner, and Paul
American Annals of the Deaf , 154(4), 338-345.
Albertini, J. & Mayer, C. (2011). Using miscue analysis to assess comprehension in deaf college
readers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education,
16(1), 35-46.
Alloway T.P. & Gathercole, S.E.(2006). Verbal and visuospatial short-term and working
memory in children: are they separable? Child Development, 77(6), 1698 1716.
Alloway, T.P., Gathercole, S.E., Kirkwood, H., & Elliott, J. (2009). The cognitive and behavioral
characteristics of children with low working memory. Child Development, 80(2), 606
621.
Altshuler, K.Z., Deming, W.E., Vollenweider, J., Rainer, J.D., & Tendler, R.(1976).
Impulsivity and profound early deafness: A cross cultural inquiry. American Annals of
the Deaf, 121(3), 331-45.
Archibald, L. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Short-term memory and working memory in
MEMORY SKILLS 45
specific language impairment. In T. P. Alloway, & S. E. Gathercole (Eds.), Working
memory and neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 139-160). New York, NY: Taylor &
Francis.
Ayres, P. (2009). State of the art research on cognitive load theory. Computers in Human
Behavior, 25(2), 253-257.
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working Memory: Looking back and looking forward.
Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 189208.
Baddeley, A. D., & Wilson, B. A. (1994). When implicit learning fails: Amnesia and the
problem of error elimination. Neuropsychologia, 32, 53-68.
Baker-Shenk, C. & Cokely, D. (1991). American Sign Language Green: A Teacher's Resource
Text on Grammar and Culture. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.
Bates, E. (1976). Language and context: Studies in the acquisition of pragmatics. New
York: Academic Press.
Bates, E., MacWhinney, B., Caselli, C., Devescovi, A., Natale, F., & Venza, V. (1984).
A cross-linguistic study of the development of sentence interpretation strategies.
Child Development, 55, 341-354.
Bavelier, D., Newport, E.L., Hall, M., Supalla, T., & Boutla, M. (2008). Ordered short-term
memory differs in signers and speakers: Implications for models of short-term memory.
Cognition, 107(2): 433459.
Bebko, J.M. (1979) Can recall differences among children be attributed to rehearsal effects?
Canadian Journal of Psychology, 33, 96-105.
Bebko, J. M. (1984). Memory and rehearsal characteristics of profoundly deaf children. Journal
of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 415-428.
MEMORY SKILLS 46
Bebko, J. M. & McKinnon, E. (1990). The language experience of deaf children: Its relation
to spontaneous rehearsal in a memory task. Child Development, 61(6) 1744-1752.
Bebko, J. M., Bell, M , Metcalfe-Haggert, A., & McKinnon, E. (1998). Language proficiency
and the prediction of spontaneous rehearsal use in children who are deaf. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 68(1) 51-69.
Beech, J. R., & Harris, M. (1997). The prelingually deaf young reader: A case of reliance
on direct lexical access? Journal of Research in Reading, 20(2), 105121.
Bellugi, .U, Klima, E.S., & Siple, P. (1975). Remembering in signs, Cognition, 3, 93-125.
Bellugi, U. & Siple, P (1974). Remembering with and without words. In F. Bresson (Ed.)
Current problems in psycholiniguistics (pp.215-235). Paris: Centre National de la
Recherche Scientifque.
Belmont, J. M., Karchmer, M. A., & Pilkonis, P. A. (1976). Instructed rehearsal strategies'
influence on deaf memory processing. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 19, 36-
47.
Benton, A. L. (1974). Revised Visual Retention Test. Clinical and experimental applications (4
th
ed.). New York: The Psychological Corporation.
Blair, F. (1957). A study of the visual memory of deaf and hearing children. American
Annals of the Deaf, 102, 254-263.
Blatto-Vallee, G. & Kelly, R.R. (2007). Visualspatial representation in mathematical problem
solving by deaf and hearing students. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education,
12(4), 432-448..
MEMORY SKILLS 47
Bonvillian, J. D., Rea, C. A., Orlansky, M. D., & Slade, L. A. (1987). The effect of sign
language rehearsal on deaf subjects' immediate and delayed recall of English word lists.
Applied Psycholinguistics, 8, 33-53.
Boutla, M., Supalla, T., Newport, E.L., & Bavelier, D. (2004). Short-term memory span:
Insights from sign language. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 9971002.
Bowey, J. A., & Patel, R. K. (1988). Metalinguistic ability and early reading achievement.
Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 367-383.
Braden, J.P. (1990). Do deaf persons have a characteristic psychometric profile on the
Wechsler Performance Scales? Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 8(4), 518-526.
Cain, K. (2006). Individual differences in children's memory and reading comprehension: An
investigation of semantic and inhibitory deficits. Memory, 14(5), 553 569.
Cain, K. & Oakhill, J.(2006).Profiles of children with specific reading comprehension
difficulties. The British Journal Of Educational Psychology, 76( 4), 683-96.
Campbell, R.(1987) The cerebral lateralization of lip-reading. In B. Dodd B & R. Campbell
(Eds.), Hearing by eye: The psychology of lip-reading (pp215-226). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Campbell, R., & Wright, H. (1988). Deafness, spelling, and rhyme: How spelling
supports written word and picture rhyming skills in deaf subjects. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology , 40, 771788.
Campbell, R., & Wright, H. (1990). Deafness and immediate memory for pictures:
MEMORY SKILLS 48
Dissociations between inner speech and the inner ear? Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 50, 259-286.
Case, R. (1977). Intellectual and linguistic development in the preschool years (final
report) Spencer Foundation.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2010). Increasing prevalence of parent-reported
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among children --- United States, 2003 and 2007.
Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 59(44);1439-1443.
Chen, Q., Zhang, M., & Zhou, X. (2006). Effects of spatial distribution of attention
during inhibition of return (IOR) on flanker interference in hearing
and congenitally deaf people. Brain Research, 1109, 117127.
Chi, M., Glaser, R. & Rees, E. (1982). Expertise in problem solving. In: R. Sternberg
(Ed.), Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence (pp. 775). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence J. Erlbaum.
Clare, L., Wilson, B. A., Carter, G., Roth, I., & Hodges, J. R. (2002). Relearning of
facename associations in early-stage Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 16, 538
547.
Clark, C.B., & Klecan-Aker, J. (1992). Therapeutic strategies for language disordered children:
The impact of visual imagery and verbal encoding in vocabulary instruction. Journal of
Childhood Communication Disorders, 14, 129-145.
Comblain, A. (1994). Working memory in Down's syndrome: Training the rehearsal
strategy. Down's Syndrome: Research and Practice, 2, 123-126.
Cormier, S.M. & Hagman, J.D. (1987). Transfer of learning: Contemporary research
MEMORY SKILLS 49
and applications. San Diego: Academic Press.
Cornett, O. (1967). Cued speech. American Annals of the Deaf, 112, 3-13.
Darwin, C.J., Turvey, M.T. & Browder, R.G. (1972). An auditory analogue of the
Sperling partial report procedure: Evidence for brief auditory storage. Cognitive.
Psychology. 3, 255267.
Davey, B., & King, S. (1990). Acquisition of word meanings from context by deaf readers.
American Annals of the Deaf, 135, 227-234.
Davies, G.M. (1980). Can memory be educated? Educational Studies, 6, 155-161.
Denh, M, (2008) Working memory and academic learning: assessment and intervention.
Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
de Jonge P. & de Jong P.F. (1996). Working memory, intelligence and reading ability in
children. Personality and individual differences, 21(6), 1007-1020.
Dickinson, D. K., McCabe, A., Anastasopoulos, L., Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., Poe, M. D.
(2003). The comprehensive language approach to early literacy: The interrelationships
among vocabulary, phonological sensitivity, and print knowledge among preschool
children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(3), 465-481.
Dodd B. (1987). Lip-reading, phonological coding and deafness. In: Dodd B., Campbell, R.
(Eds.) Hearing by eye: The psychology of lipreading(pp 177-189).. London: Lawrence
Erlbaum Ass. Ltd.
Dodd B, & Hermelin B. (1977). Phonological coding by the prelinguistically deaf.
Perception and Psychophysics 21, 413417.
Drasgow, E., & Paul, P. (1995). A critical analysis of the use of MCE systems with deaf
MEMORY SKILLS 50
students: A review of the literature. Association of Canadian Educators of the Hearing
Impaired, 21, 8093.
Duranti, A., & Ochs, E. (1979). Left-dislocation in Italian conversation. In T. Givon (Ed.)
Syntax and semantics. vol. 12: Discourse and Syntax (pp 377-416) New York: Academic
Press.
Dye, M.W., Hauser, P.C, & Bavelier, D. (2008). Visual attention in deaf children and
adults. In M. Marschark & P.C, Hauser (Eds.) Deaf Cognition: Foundations and
outcomes (pp. 250-263). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Dyer, A., MacSweeney, M., Szczerbinski, M., Green, L., & Campbell, R. (2003).
Predictors of reading delay in deaf adolescents: The relative contributions of rapid
automatized naming speed and phonological awareness and decoding. Journal of Deaf
Studies and Deaf Education, 8, 215229.
Eldredge, N. (1984). The impact of hearing loss on the development of visual perception:
Development of trends in graphic strategies used to copy the Rey-Osterrieth Complex
Figure. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Oregon State University, Corvallis.
Eldredge, N., & Zhang, N. (1988). A cross cultural view of cognitive development: Visual
perception, memory, and deafness in the United States, and the People’s Republic
of China. In D. Watson, G. Long, M. Taff-Watson, & M.Harvey (Eds.), Two decades of
excellence: A foundation for the future, Monograph No. 14 (pp. 163181). Little Rock,
AR: American Deaf and Rehabilitation Association.
Emmorey, K., Klima, E., & Hickok, G. (1998). Mental rotation within linguistic and
nonlinguistic domains in users of American sign language. Cognition, 68, 221-246.
Emmorey, K_, Kosslyn, S. M., & Bellugi, U. (1993). Visual imagery and visual spatial language:
MEMORY SKILLS 51
Enhanced imagery abilities in deaf and hearing ASL signers. Cognition, 46, 139-181.
Emmorey, K., & Kosslyn, S. (1996). Enhanced image generation abilities in deaf signers:
A right hemisphere effect. Brain and Cognition, 32, 2844.
Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 11, 19-23.
Engle, R. W., Kane, M. J., & Tuholski, S. W. (1999). Individual differences in working
memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid
intelligence and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In Miyake, A. & Shah, P. (Eds.),
Models of working memory: mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control
(pp.102-134). London: Cambridge Press.
Engle, R. W., Carullo, J. J., & Collins, K. W. (1991). Individual differences in the role of
working memory in comprehension and following directions. Journal of Educational
Research, 84, 253-262.
Eslinger, P. (2002). Neuropsychological Interventions: Clinical research and practice.
New York: The Guilford Press.
Feifer, S. G. & DeFina, PA. (2000). The neuropsychology of reading disorders:
Diagnosis and intervention. Middletown, MD: School Neuropsychology Press, LLC.
Felser, C. & Clahsen, H. (2009). Grammatical processing of spoken language in child
and adult language learners. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 38(3) 305-319.
Flaherty, M. & Moran, A. (2001). Memory span for Arabic numerals and digit words in Japanese
kanji in deaf signers. Japanese Psychological Research, 43(2), 6371.
Flaherty, M., & Moran, A. (2004). Deaf signers who know Japanese remember words and
numbers more effectively than deaf signers who know English. American Annals of the
MEMORY SKILLS 52
Deaf, 149(1), 3945.
Flavell, J. H., Beach, D. R., & Chinsky, J. M. (1966). Spontaneous verbal rehearsal in a memory
task as a function of age. Child Development, 37,283-299.
Fletcher, J.M., Lyon, G.R., Fuchs, L.S., & Barnes, M.A. (2007) Learning disabilities:
From identification to intervention. New York: Guilford.
Gallaudet Research Institute. (1996). Stanford Achievement Test, 9th Edition, Form S,
Norms Booklet for Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Students. Washington, DC: Gallaudet
University.
Gallaudet Research Institute. (2008). Regional and national summary report of data from
the 200708 annual survey of deaf and hard of hearing children and youth. Washington,
DC: Gallaudet University.
Gathercole, S.E. & Alloway, T.P. (2008). Working memory and learning: A practical
guide. London: Sage Publication
Gathercole, S.E., Lamont, E., & Alloway T.P. (2006). Working memory in the
classroom. In Pickering, S. (Ed.) Working memory and Education (pp. 219-240). San
Diego: Academic Press.
Gathercole, S.E. & Pickering, S.J. (2000). Working memory deficits in children with low
achievements in the national curriculum at seven years of age. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 70, 177194.
Gathercole, S.E., Pickering, S.J., Knight, C. & Stegmann, Z. (2004). Working memory skills and
educational attainment: Evidence from National Curriculum assessments at 7 and 14
years of age. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 40, 116.
MEMORY SKILLS 53
Geary, D.C., Hoard, M.K., Nugent, L. & Byrd-Craven, J. (2007). Development of number
line representations in children with mathematical learning disability. Developmental
Neuropsychology, 33(3), 277299.
Geraci, C., Gozzi, M., Papagno, C., Cecchetto,C. (2008). How grammar can cope with
limited short-term memory: simultaneity and seriality in sign languages. Cognition.
106(2), 780-804. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.04.014.
Gibson, I. (2003).Teaching strategies used to develop short-term memory in deaf
children. British Association of Teachers of the Deaf. Retrieved from
http://www.batod.org.uk/content/articles/mgwts/teachingstrategiesmgwts.pdf .
Gill, C.B., Klecan-Aker, J. Roberts, T., Fredenburg, K.A. (2003). Following directions:
Rehearsal and visualizatin strategies for children with specific language impairment.
Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 19(1), 85-103.
Gillette, J., Gleitman, H., Gleitman, L., & Lederer, A.(1999). Human simulations of vocabulary
learning. Cognition 73, 135-176.
Goldin-Meadow, S. (1999). What children contribute to language-learning. Science Progress. 82
(1), 89-102.
Goldin-Meadow,S. & Mylander, C. (1990). The role of parental input in the development
of a morphological system. Journal of Child Language. 17(3):527-63.
Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. E. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Grushkin, D. (1998). Why shouldn’t Sam read: Towards a new paradigm for literacy
and the deaf. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 3, 179204.
MEMORY SKILLS 54
Hamilton, H. (1984). Cheremic perception by deaf children. Sign Language Studies, 42. 23-30.
Hamilton, H. (1985). Linguistic encoding and adult-child interaction. In D. S.Martin (Ed.),
Cognition, Education and Deafness: Directions for Research and Instruction
(pp. 91-93). Washington: Gallaudet College Press.
Hamilton, H. (1987). Grandfather Moose: Sign language nursery rhymes. Perspectives.
X.
Hamilton, H. (1988). Grandfather Moose: Rhymes, games, and chants in sign language.
Los Alamitos, CA: Modern Signs Press.
Hamilton, H. & Holzman, T. (1989). Linguistic encoding in short-term memory as a
function of stimulus type. Memory and Cognition. 17(5), 542-550.
Hamilton, H. & Jones , G. (1989). The box: A low-error method for teaching English
skills. Volta Review, 91(1), 19-26.
Hanson, V. L. (1982). Short-term recall by deaf signers of American Sign Language:
Implications of encoding strategy for order recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory and Cognition, 8, 572-583.
Hanson, V. L. (1989). Phonology and reading: Evidence from profoundly deaf readers. In D.
Shankweiler & I. Liberman (Eds.), Phonology and reading disability: Solving the reading
puzzle (pp. 6989). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.
Hanson V. (1990). Recall of order information by deaf signers: Phonetic coding in temporal
order recall. Haskins Laboratories Status Report on Speech Research, SR-103/104, 165-
172
MEMORY SKILLS 55
Hanson, V. (1991). Phonological processing without sound. In S. Brady & D. Shankweiler
(Eds.),
Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabel Y. Liberman (pp.153-161).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hanson, V. L., &: Lichtenstein, E. H. (1990). Short-term memory coding by deaf signers: The
primary language coding hypothesis reconsidered. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 211-224.
Harris, M., & Beech, J. R. (1998). Implicit phonological awareness and early reading
development in pre-lingually deaf children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf
Education, 3(3), 205216.
Harris, M., & Moreno, C. (2004). Deaf children’s use of phonological coding: Evidence
from reading, spelling and working memory. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf
Education, 9, 253268.
Harris, M., & Moreno, C. (2006). Speech reading and learning to read: A comparison of
8-year-old profoundly deaf children with good and poor reading ability. Journal of Deaf
Studies and Deaf Education, 11(2), 189201.
Hauser, P., Dye, M.W.G., Cohen, J., & Bavelier, D. (2007). Visual constructive
and visual-motor skills in deaf native signers. Journal of Deaf Studies and
Deaf Education, 12 (2), 148 157.
Henry, L. & Winfield, J. (2010). Working memory and educational achievement in children with
intellectual impairments. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 54, 354-365.
Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Treiman, R. (1982). Recoding in silent reading: Can the deaf child translate
MEMORY SKILLS 56
print into a more manageable form. Volta Review, 84, 72-82.
Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (1996). The origins of grammar: Evidence from early
language comprehension. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Holmes, J., Gathercole, S.E., & Dunning, D.L. (2009). Adaptive training leads to
sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Developmental Science, 12
(4), 915.
Hyerle, D. & Yeager, C. (2007). Thinking Maps: A Language for Learning. Cary, NC:
Thinking Maps, Inc.
Jarvis H.L & Gathercole S.E.(2003). Verbal and non-verbal working
memory and achievements on National Curriculum tests at 11 and
14 years of age. Educational and Child Psychology, 20(3) 123-140.
Jelsma, O., van Merrienboer, J. J. G., & Bijlstra, J. P. (1990). The ADAPT design model:
Towards instructional control of transfer. Instructional Science, 19, 89-120.
Johns, J. (2009). Basic Reading Inventory. Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt.
Juel C., Griffith P. L. & Gough P. B. (1986). Acquisition of literacy: A longitudinal study
of children in first and second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 243-255.
Kelly, D , Kelly, B., Jones, M., Moulton, N., Verhulst, S., & Bell, S. (1993). Attention deficits in
children and adolescents with hearing loss. American Journal of Diseases of Children,
147, 737-741.
Kelly, L. (1993). Recall of English function words and inflections by skilled and average
deaf readers. American Annals of the Deaf, 138(3), 288-296.
Kelly, M., & Braden J.P. (1990). Criterion validity of the WISC-R performance scale
MEMORY SKILLS 57
with the Stanford achievement test -Hearing Impaired edition. Journal of School
Psychology, 28, 147-151.
Kennison, S. M., Sieck, J. P., & Briesch, K. A. (2003). Evidence for a late occurring effect of
phoneme repetition in silent reading. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32, 297-312.
Klima, E.S., & Bellugi, U. (1979) The signs of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlstrom, K., et al.
(2005). Computerized training of working memory in children
with ADHDA randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 177186.
Klingberg, T., Forssberg, H., & Westerberg, H. (2002). Training of working memory in
children with ADHD. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 781
791.
Kluwin, T. N. (1981). The grammaticality of manual representations of English.
American Annals of the Deaf, 126, 417-421.
Koo, D., Crain, K., LaSasso, C. and Eden G.F. (2008). Phonological Awareness and Short Term
Memory in Hearing and Deaf Individuals of Different Communication Backgrounds.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1145, 83-99.
Kottler, E., Kottler, J.A., Street, C. (2007). Children with Limited English: Teaching Strategies
for the Regular Classroom. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin.
MEMORY SKILLS 58
Krakow, R. A. & Hanson, V.L. (1985). Deaf signers and serial recall in the visual
modality: memory of signs, fingerspelling and print. Memory and Cognition, 13, 265-
272.
Kyle, J.G. (1981). Signs and memory: The search for the code. In B. Woll, J. Kyle, & M.
Deucaher (Eds.) Perspectives on British Sign Language and Deafness (pp.71-88).
London: Croon Helm.
Kyle, F. E., & Harris, M. (2006). Concurrent correlates and predictors of reading and
spelling achievement in deaf and hearing school children. Journal of Deaf Studies and
Deaf Education, 11(3), 273288.
Kyle, J.G & Harris, M (2010). Predictors of reading development in deaf children: A 3-
year longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 229-243.
Lan, L. (2005). The growing prosperity of on-line dictionaries. English Today, 21(3), 16-21.
Lang, H. & Pagliaro, C (2010). Factors predicting recall of mathematics terms by deaf
students: Implications for teaching. Journal of Deaf Studies Deaf Education.
12(4), 449-460.
Larr, A. L. (1956). Perceptual and conceptual ability of residential school deaf children.
Exceptional Children, 23, 63 66.
LaSasso, C , & Davey, B. (1987). The relationship between lexical knowledge and reading
comprehension for prelingually, profoundly hearing-impaired students. Volta Review, 89,
211-220.
Lederberg, A. (2006). Language development of deaf children with hearing parents, In K.
Brown, (Ed.) Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics (pp 361-368). Oxford: Elsevier.
MEMORY SKILLS 59
Lee, D., & Riccio, C. A. (2005). Understanding and implementing cognitive
Neuropsychological retraining. In R. C. D’Amato, E. Fletcher-Janzen, & C. R. Reynolds
(Eds.) Handbook of school neuropsychology (pp. 701720). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Leutzinger, M.R. (2002). Use of the WAIS III performance scale with deaf adults.
Dissertation abstracts international: the sciences and engineering, 63 (2 B), 1036.
Leybaert, J., & Alegria, J. (1995). Spelling development in deaf and hearing children.
Evidence for use of morpho-phonological regularities in French. Reading and Writing, 7,
89109.
Lesgold, A.M. & Perfetti, C.A. (1978). Interactive processes in reading comprehension.
Discourse Processes, 1, 323336.
Liben, L. S. (1979). Free recall by deaf and hearing children: Semantic clustering and
recall in trained and untrained groups. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27,
105119.
Liben, L. & Drury, A. (1977). Short-term memory in deaf and hearing children in relation
to stimulus characteristics. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 24, 60-73.
Lichtenstein, E. H. (1985). Deaf working memory processes and English language skills.
In D. S.Martin (Ed.), Cognition, education, and deafness: Directions for research and
instruction (pp. 111-114). Washington, D.C.: Gallaudet College Press.
Lichtenstein, S. (1998). The relationship between reading processes and English skills
of college deaf students. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 3, 80-134.
Lindner, K.(2003). The development of sentence-interpretation strategies in monolingual
German-learning children with and without specific language impairment.
Linguistics, 41(2), 213- 242.
MEMORY SKILLS 60
Logan, K., Mayberry, M., & Fletcher, J. (1996). The short-term memory of profoundly
deaf people for words, signs, and abstract spatial stimuli. Applied Cognitive Psychology,
10, 10-119.
Loke, W. H. & Song, S. (1991). Central and peripheral visual processing in
hearing and non-hearing individuals. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,
29(5), 437 440.
Luckner, J., Bowen, S., & Carter, K. (2001). Visual teaching strategies for students who are deaf
or hard of hearing. Teaching Exceptional Children, 33, 38-44.
Luetke-Stahlman, B. (1988). Documenting syntactically and semantically incomplete
bimodal input to hearing-impaired subjects. American Annals of the Deaf, 133(3), 230-
234.
Luetke-Stahlman, B. (1991) Following the rules: Consistency in sign. Journal of Speech
and Hearing Research. 34, 1293-1298.
Ma, W., Golinkoff, R.M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., McDonough, C., & Tardif, T. (2008). Imageability
predicts the age of acquisition of verbs in Chinese children. Journal of Child Language,
36, 405423.
MacSweeney, M. (1998). Short-term memory processes and reading by deaf children.
Proceedings of the Second ACFOS International Colloquium, Paris 1998: Deafness and
Access to Written Language, 1, 165-173.
Maller, S. J., & Ferron, J. (1997). WISC III factor invariance across deaf and
standardization samples. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 57,
987 994.
Marmor, G. S., & Petitto, L. (1979). Simultaneous communication in the classroom:
MEMORY SKILLS 61
How well is English grammar represented? Sign Language Studies,
23, 99-136.
Marschark, M. (1997). Raising and educating a deaf child. New York: Oxford University Press.
Marschark, M. (2006). Intellectual functioning of deaf adults and children:
Answers and questions. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 7089.
doi:10.1080/09541440500216028.
Marschark M, & Harris M. (1996). Success and failure in learning to read: The special case of
deaf children, In C. Cornoldi & J. Oakhill (Eds.), Reading comprehension difficulties:
Processes and intervention (pp 279300). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Marschark, M., Lang, H., & Albertini, J. (2002). Educating deaf students: From research
to practice. New York: Oxford University Press.
Marschark, M., Pelz, J., Convertino, C., Sapere, P., Arndt, M. E.,
& Seewagen, R. (2005). Classroom interpreting and visual information processing in
mainstream education for deaf students: Live or Memorex? American Educational
Research Journal, 42, 727762. doi:10.3102/00028312042004727.
Marschark, M., Sapere, P., Convertino, C. M., Mayer, C., Wauters L., & Sarchet, T. (2009). Are
deaf students' reading challenges really about reading? American Annals of the Deaf,
154(4), 357-70.
Marschark, M. & Wauters, L. (2008). Language comprehension and learning by deaf
students. In M. Marschark, M. & P.C. Hauser ( Eds.), Deaf cognition: Foundations and
outcomes (pp.309-350). New York: Oxford University Press.
MEMORY SKILLS 62
Mastropieri, M.E., & Scruggs, T.E. (1998). Enhancing school success with mnemonic
strategies. Intervention in School and Clinic, 33, 201-208.
Mather, N., & Jaffe, L. E. (2002). Woodcock-Johnson III: Reports, recommendations,
and strategies. New York: Wiley.
Matthews, T.J., & Reich, C.F. (1993). Constraints on communication in classrooms for
the deaf. American Annals of the Deaf, 138, 14-18.
Mayberry, R.I. & Fischer, S.D. (1989). Looking through phonological shape to lexical
meaning: The bottleneck of non-native sign language processing. Memory and Cognition,
17 (6), 740-754.
Mayberry, R., del Giudice, A.A. & Lieberman, A.M. (2011). Reading achievement in
relation to phonological coding and awareness in deaf readers: a meta-analysis. Journal
of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 16(2), 164-188
Mayer, P., & Lowenbraun, S. (1990). Total communication use among elementary teachers of
hearing-impaired children. American Annals of the Deaf, 135, 257-263.
McCutchen, D., & Perfetti, C. A. (1982). The visual tongue-twister effect: Phonological
activation in silent reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 672
687.
McElree, B., Foraker, S., & Dyer, L. (2003). Memory structures that subserve sentence
comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 67-91.
McKay, B. E. (1952). An exploratory study of the psychological effects of severe
hearing impairment. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University.
MEMORY SKILLS 63
McKee, D. (1988). An analysis of specialized cognitive functions in deaf and hearing
signers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pittsburgh.
McLean, J.F. & Hitch, G.J. (1999). Working memory impairments in children with
specific arithmetic difficulties. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 74, 240260.
McNamara, D. S., & Scott, J. L. (2001). Working memory capacity and strategy use. Memory &
Cognition, 29, 10-17.
Meadow, K. P. (1976). Behavior problems of deaf children. In H. S. Schlesinger
and K. P. Meadow (Eds.), Studies of family interaction, language acquisition
and deafness (pp. 257293). San Francisco: University of California.
Meadow-Orleans, K. P. (2001) Research and deaf education: Moving ahead while
glancing back. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 6(2), 143 -148.
Merrills, J. D., Underwood, G., & Wood, D. J. (1994). The word recognition skills of
profoundly, prelingually deaf children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12,
365384.
Mezzacappa, E. & Buckner, J.C, (2010).Working memory training for children with
attention problems or hyperactivity: A school-based pilot study. School Mental Health,
Feb., 1-7.
Minear, M., & Shah, P. (2006). Sources of working memory deficits in children and possibilities
for remediation. In S. J. Pickering (Ed.), Working memory and education. (pp. 273-307).
London: Elsevier.
Mitchell, T. V. & Quittner, A. L. (1996). Multimethod study of attention and
behavior problems in hearing-impaired children. Journal of Clinical
MEMORY SKILLS 64
Child Psychology, 25(1), 83 96.
Montgomery, J. (2003). Working memory and comprehension in children with specific
language impairment: What we know so far. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36,
221-231.
Moores, D. F. (2001). Educating the deaf: Psychology, principles, and practices (5th
ed.). Boston: Houghton-Mifflin Co.
Moores, D. F. (2003). Short term memory, morphology, and reading (editorial).
American Annals of the Deaf, 148(1), 34.
Myklebust, H. R. & Brutten, M. A. (1953). A study of the visual perception of
deaf children. Acta Otolaryngologica, Supplement 105.
Nagy, W., Anderson, R., Schommer, M., Scott, J., & Stallman, A. (1989). Morphological
families in the internal lexicon. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 262282.
Narr, R. F. (2008). Phonological awareness and decoding in deaf/hard-of-hearing
students who use Visual Phonics. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 13(3),
405416.
Nielsen, D. C., & Luetke-Stahlman, B. (2002). Phonological awareness: One key to the reading
proficiency of deaf children. American Annals of the Deaf, 147, 1119.
Nunes, T. (2006). Promoting deaf children’s mathematical reasoning by using the visual-
spatial strengths. In R.J. Sternberg & R. F. Subotnik (Eds.) Optimizing Student Success
in School with the Other Three Rs: Reasoning, Resilience, and Responsibility (pp. 75-94).
Greenwich, CN: IAP-Information Age.
O’Donnell, A. M., & Adenwalla, D. (1991). Using cooperative learning and concept maps
with deaf college students. In Martin, D. S. (ed.), Advances in Cognition, Learning, and
MEMORY SKILLS 65
Deafness (pp. 348355). Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.
Olsson, J. E., & Furth, H. G. (1966). Visual memory-span in the deaf. American Journal of
Psychology, 79(3) , 480-484.
Paas, F. (1992). Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in
statistics: A cognitive load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 429-434.
Padden, C., & Hanson, V. (2000). Search for the missing link: The development of skilled
reading in deaf children. In K. Emmorey & H. Lane (Eds), The signs of language
revisited: An anthology to honor Ursula Bellugi and Edward Klima (pp. 435447).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Parasnis, I., & Kirk, U. (2004). The Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test performance: Evidence
for strategy differences in deaf signers. Unpublished manuscript, Rochester Institute of
Technology
Parasnis, I., Samar, V., & Berent, G. (2003). Deaf adults without attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder display reduced perceptual sensitivity and elevated impulsivity on
the Test of Variables of Attention (T.O.V.A.). Journal of Speech Language and Hearing
Research, 46, 1166-1183.
Parasnis, I., Samar, V. J., Bettger, J. G., & Sathe, K. (1996). Does deafness lead to
enhancement of visual spatial cognition in children? Negative evidence from deaf
nonsigners. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 1(2), 145-152.
Paul, P.V. (1996). Reading vocabulary knowledge and deafness. Journal of Deaf Studies and
Deaf Education. 1(1), 3-15.
Paul, P., & Gustafson, G. (1991). Comprehension of high frequency multi-meaning words by
students with hearing impairment. Remedial and Special Education (RASE), 12
MEMORY SKILLS 66
(4), 52-62.
Paul, P., & O'Rourke, J. (1988). Multimeaning words and reading comprehension: Implications
for special education students. Remedial and Special Education (RASE), 9 (3), 42-52.
Paul, P.V., Wang, Y, Trezek, B. J., & Luckner, J. L., (2009). Phonology is
necessary, but not sufficient: a rejoinder. American Annals of the Deaf, 4, 34656.
Perfetti, C.C., & Lesgold, A.A. (1978). Discourse comprehension and sources of individual
differences. In M. Just and P. Carpenter (Eds.) Cognitive processes in comprehension
(pp141-183). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.
Perfetti, C. A., & McCutchen, D. (1982). Speech processes in reading. In N. Lass (Ed.), Speech
and language: Advances in basic research and practice (Vol. 7, pp. 237269). New
York, NY: Academic Press.
Perfetti, C. A., & Sandak, R. (2000). Reading optimally builds on spoken language:
Implicationsfor deaf readers. Journal of Deaf Studies & Deaf Education, 5(1), 32-50.
Pickering, S.J. (2009). Working memory and education. London: Academic Press.
Pintner, R., & Patterson, D. G. (1917). Psychological tests for deaf children. Volta
Review, 19, 661667.
Prifitera, A., Saklofske, D. H., & Weiss, L. G. (Eds.). (2005). WISC-IV clinical use and
interpretation:Scientist-practitioner perspectives. New York: Elsevier.
Proksch, J., & Bavelier, D. (2002). Changes in the spatial distribution of visual attention
after early deafness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 687-701.
Qi, S., & Mitchell, R. E. (2007). Large-scale academic achievement
MEMORY SKILLS 67
testing of deaf and hard-of-hearing students: Past, present, and future. Paper presented at
the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association. Chicago, IL.
Rapport M. D, Alderson RM, Kofler MJ, Sarver DE, Bolden J, Sims V.(2008). Working memory
deficits in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): the contribution of
central executive and subsystem processes. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
36(6), 825-837.
Reyes, I. (2003). A study of sentence interpretation in Spanish monolingual children.
First Language. 23(69) 285-309.
Risley, B. & Hart, T.R. (2002). Meaningful Differences in the Everyday
Experience of Young American Children. Baltimore: Brookes.
Press.
Ritchie, D. & Karge, B. D. (1996). Making information memorable: Enhanced
knowledge retention and recall through the elaboration process. Preventing School
Failure, 41(1), 28-33.
Roberts, M.J. (2007). Integrating the Mind: Domain General versus Domain Specific
Processes in Higher Cognition. New York: Psychology.
Rudner M, & Rönnberg J. (2008). Explicit processing demands reveal language modality-
specific organization of working memory. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education.
13(4), 466-484.
Sadoski, M. and Willson, V. (2006). Effects of a theoretically based large-scale
reading intervention in a multicultural urban school district. American Educational
Research Journal, 43(1), 137-154...
MEMORY SKILLS 68
Schimmel, C., Edwards, S, & Prickett, H. (1999) Reading?...Pah! (I got it.) American
Annals of the Deaf, 144 (4), 298-308.
Schimmel, C. & Edwards, S. (2003) Literacy strategies for the classroom: putting Bi-Bi
theory into practice. Odyssey, 5(2), 58-63.
Shand, M.A. (1982). Sign-based short-term coding of American sign language signs and printed
English words by congenitally deaf signers. Cognitive Psychology, 14(1), 1-12.
Shand, M. A., & Klima, E. S. (1981). Nonauditory suffii effects in congenitally deaf signers of
American Sign Language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and
Memory, 7, 464-474.
Singley, M. K. and Anderson, J. R. (1989). The transfer of cognitive skill. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Snow, C. E. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies.
Journal of Child Language, 4, l-22.
Snow, C.E., Tabors, P.O., & Dickinson. D.K. (2001). Language development in the
preschool years. In D.K. Dickinson & P.O. Tabors (Eds.) Beginning literacy with
language (pp. 1-26). Baltimore: Brookes.
Sperber, D., Premack, D., & Premack, A.J. (1995). Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary
debate New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Stewart, J.H. (1981). Wechsler Performance IQ scores and social behaviors of hearing-
impaired students. The Volta Review, 83(4), 215-222.
Stokoe, W. C. (1960). Sign Language Structure: An Outline of the Visual Communication
Systems of the American Deaf, Studies in linguistics: Occasional papers (No. 8).
Buffalo: Dept. of Anthropology and Linguistics, University of Buffalo.
MEMORY SKILLS 69
Stroenher, H & Nelson, H. (1974). The young child’s development of sentence
comprehension: Influence of event probability, nonverbal context, syntactic form, and
strategies. Child Language. 45, 564-576.
Strong, M., & Prinz, P. (2000). Is American Sign Language skill related to English
literacy? In C. Chamberlain, J. Morford, & R.I. Mayberry (Eds.) Language acquisition
by eye (pp. 131-141). Mahwah, NJ: Earlbaum.
Swanson, H. L., Kehler, P., & Jerman, O. (2010). Working memory, strategy knowledge, and
strategy instruction in children with reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
43(1), 24-48.
Swanson, H. L., Zheng, X., & Jerman, O. (2009). Working memory, short-term memory, and
reading disabilities: A selective meta-analysis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42(3),
260-287.
Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving, Cognitive Science 12: 257
285.
Todman, J., & Seedhouse, E. (1994). Visual-action code processing by deaf and hearing
children. Language and Cognitive Processes, 9, 129-141.
Tomlinson-Keasey, C., & Smith-Winberry, C. (1990). Cognitive consequences of
congenital deafness. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 151(1), 103-115.
Treiman, R. & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1983) Silent Reading: Insights from second generation
congenitally deaf readers. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 39-65.
Transler, C. & Reitsma, P. (2005). Phonological coding in reading of deaf children:
MEMORY SKILLS 70
Pseudohomophone effects in lexical decision. British Journal of Developmental
Psychology, 23(4) , 525542.
Traxler, C.B. (2000). The Stanford Achievement Test, 9th edition: National norming and
performance standards for deaf and hard of hearing students. Journal of Deaf Studies and
Deaf Education, 5, 337348.
Waddy-Smith, B., & Wilson, V. (2003). See that sound! Visual Phonics helps deaf and
hard of hearing students develop reading skills. Odyssey: New Directions in Deaf
Education, 4(3), 1417.
Wang, Y., Trezek, B., Luckner, J., & Paul, P. (2008). The role of phonology and
phonological-related skills in reading instruction for participants who are deaf or
hard-of-hearing. American Annals of the Deaf, 153(4), 396-407.
Weaver, K., Hamilton, H., Bruckman, A.S. & Starner, T. (2010). Improving the
Language Ability of Deaf Signing Children through an Interactive American Sign
Language-Based Video Game. Paper presented at the International Conference on
Learning Sciences, June, 2010.
Werner, H. & Strauss, A. A. (1941). Pathology of figure-background relation in
the child. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 36, 236 248.
Willis, C. S., & Gathercole, S. E. (2001). Phonological short-term memory
contributions to sentence processing in young children. Memory, 9, 349-364.
Wilson, M, Bettger, J.G., Niculae, I., & Klima, E.S. (1997). Modaility of language shapes
working memory: Evidence from digit span and spatial span in ASL signers. Journal of
Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2, 152-162.
MEMORY SKILLS 71
Wilson, M., & Emmorey, K. (1997). The visuo-spatial 'phonological loop' in working
memory: Evidence from American Sign Language. Memory and Cognition, 25 (3), 313-
320.
Wilson, M., & Emmorey, K. (1998). A 'word length effect' for sign language: Further evidence
for the role of language in structuring working memory. Memory and Cognition, 26 (3),
584-590.
Wolfe J. (2000).Visual attention. In: K.K., De Valois, (Ed.). Seeing. 2nd ed. (pp. 335-386). San
Diego, CA:Academic Press.
Woodward, J. and Allen, T. (1988). Classroom use of artificial sign systems by teachers.
Sign Language Studies, 61, 405-418.
MEMORY SKILLS 72
MEMORY SKILLS 73